Polioencephalitis and the Brain Fatigue Generator Model Of Post-Viral Fatigue Syndromes
Richard L. Bruno, HD, PhD, et al.
Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, 1996; 2: 5-27.
Abstract
Fatigue is the most commonly reported and most debilitating Post-Polio Sequelae (PPS) affecting millions of polio survivors world-wide. Post- polio fatigue is associated with:
Subjective reports of difficulty with attention, cognition, word-finding and maintaining wakefulness;
Clinically significant deficits on neuropsychological tests of information processing speed and attention;
Gray and white matter hyperintensities in the reticular activating system on magnetic resonance imaging of the brain;
Neuroendocrine evidence of impaired activation of the hypothalamus.
Many of these findings are identical to those documented following a variety of viral encephalitides, besides acute poliovirus infection including lethargic encephalitis, Iceland Disease, myalgic encephalomyelitis and, most recently, Chronic Fatigue Syndrome.
The clinical, historic, neuropsychologic, neuroanatomic and physiologic parallels between poliovirus infection, post-polio fatigue and post viral fatigue syndromes (PVFS) will be explored in an attempt to describe the pathophysiology of PVFS. The disinhibition of a putative Brain Fatigue Generator will be implicated as a cause of the subjective symptoms and objective signs that accompany PVFS. The results of a pilot placebo-controlled study of a dopamine 2 receptor agonist to treat post-polio fatigue will also be described.
Fatigue is the most commonly reported, most debilitating and least studied Post-Polio Sequelae (PPS) affecting millions of polio survivors throughout the world. In two national surveys of American polio survivors, 91% reported new or increased fatigue, 41% reported fatigue significantly interfering with performing or completing work and 25% reported fatigue interfering with self-care activities. 1 Fatigue was reported to be triggered or exacerbated by physical overexertion in 92% and by emotional stress in 61%.
Importantly, polio survivors differentiate between the physical tiredness and decreased endurance they associate with new muscles weakness, and a "brain fatigue" that is characterized by problems with attention and cognition. Between 70% and 96% of American polio survivors reported that fatigue was accompanied by problems with concentration, memory, attention, word-finding, maintaining wakefulness and thinking clearly, with 77% percent reporting at least moderate to severe difficulty with these problems. 2
These reports of late-onset "brain fatigue" are reminiscent of the persistent drowsiness, fatigue and fleeting attention that frequently followed the acute poliovirus infection. Clinical reports written during the polio epidemics documented "drowsiness," lethargy, apathy, slowing of the EEG, prolonged somnolence, "fatigability and fleeting attention" for months after the acute episode of paralytic and non-paralytic polio. 3-8 Such symptoms should have been expected since poliovirus produced an encephalitis, whether paralysis was present or not, consistently and often severely damaging brain areas responsible for cortical activation - the midbrain reticular formation, thalamus, locus ceruleus, posterior and paraventricular hypothalamus (i.e., the reticular activating system (RAS)) - and for directed attention, i.e., the putamen, globus pallidus and the substantia nigra. 5-17
Post-Polio Fatigue and Post-Viral Fatigue Syndromes
Both acute and late-onset post-polio fatigue are also reminiscent of Myalgic Encephalomyelitis (ME), Iceland Disease and Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS). 18-19 There are a number of clinical, historical, anatomical and physiological parallels between polio and these putative post viral fatigue syndromes (PVFS).
Myalgic Encephalomyelitis. Between 1934 and 1954 there were over a dozen outbreaks of a syndrome that was at first diagnosed as poliomyelitis, then as "abortive" or "atypical" poliomyelitis and finally as Myalgic Encephalomyelitis (ME). 18 Like poliomyelitis, initial symptoms of ME included headache, neck pain, low-grade fever and myalgia that were often followed by paresis. Patients also demonstrated hypersomnolence, slowing of the EEG and "conspicuous changes in their levels of concentration" that persisted for months after the acute illness. 18 Unlike poliomyelitis, there were frequent complaints of numbness or parasthesias, usually no respiratory involvement, infrequent paralysis or muscle atrophy and almost invariably no fatalities. Recovery from these acute symptoms ME sometimes required months and most patients were left with a marked "exhaustion and fatiguability" that were "always made worse by exercise [and] emotional stress."18- 20 Patients continued to demonstrate fatigue, hypersomnolence, impaired concentration and reported "an inordinate desire to sleep," impaired naming/word finding difficulty, that they were "not as quick or incisive in thought as before, [had] a decreased ability to learn and a decline in their short-term memory for years after the acute episode. 18
Despite the differences between poliomyelitis and ME, and the fact that poliovirus was never isolated from ME patients, some association with the poliovirus was suggested. Of the more than one dozen ME outbreaks before the introduction of the Salk vaccine, nine occurred during or immediately after outbreaks of polio and several involved hospital staff who cared for polio patients. 21-28
Iceland Disease. The PVFS epidemic most intimately linked with polio occurred in Akureyri, Iceland in 1948. Following the diagnosis of two unequivocal cases of poliomyelitis, patients began presenting with fever, myalgia and paresis that were at first diagnosed as having poliomyelitis. 29 This diagnosis was discarded as patients reported additional symptoms atypical of polio, including parasthesias, numbness, "nervousness" and "general tiredness" both acutely and for months after the acute episode. Six years after the original infection, 72% complained of "nervousness and general tiredness” while 21% reported "loss of memory." As in ME, poliovirus was never isolated from any of these patients.
Two alternatives were suggested for the cause of this constellation of symptoms that Sigurdsson called “Akureyri Disease” but has more commonly been referred to as Iceland Disease (ID): "Either a strain of poliomyelitis virus with unusual pathologic properties and of low virulence was responsible for this epidemic or...some unknown neurotropic virus has been present." 30 Support for an "unusual" poliovirus as the cause came in 1955 with an "extensive epidemic" of poliomyelitis, resulting from Type I poliovirus infection, that coincided with and was followed by outbreaks of ID. 31 Remarkably, two Icelandic cities in which ID outbreaks were reported in 1955, as well as the area affected by the 1948 Akureyri Disease epidemic, were untouched by poliomyelitis. None of the children tested in the two new ID-affected cities, and only 13% of the children in Akureyri, showed antibodies to Type I poliovirus as opposed to 86% of the children tested in the polio epidemic areas. Further, following poliovirus immunization, children in one of the ID- affected cities demonstrated antibody titres to Type II and Type III poliovirus that were four and twenty-five times higher, respectively, than titers in a city where ID had not been reported. The authors concluded that Type I poliovirus was not related to the occurrence of ID but that inhabitants of the ID-affected areas had previously been exposed to an agent immunologically similar to Type III poliovirus.
Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS). Like post-polio fatigue, ME, and ID, CFS is associated with complaints of chronic fatigue, triggered or exacerbated by physical exertion and emotional stress, that have been associated with impaired concentration, memory and word finding ability, and an excess of irregular slow wave activity on EEG. (cf. 3; 2, 32-36)
Subjective difficulties with attention in CFS patients and polio survivors with fatigue have been corroborated by the documentation of clinical abnormalities on neuropsychological testing. CFS patients and polio survivors with severe fatigue demonstrated clinically significant impairments of attention and information processing speed. 17, 37-40 In spite of these marked impairments of attention, polio survivors and CFS patients in several studies have been shown to be within the high normal on measures of higher level cognitive processes and I.Q. 1,33,40 Further, despite the high frequency of subjective complaints of memory impairment in CFS patients and in 87% of polio survivors reporting fatigue, verbal memory has been shown to be intact on testing in both groups. 2, 38-40
In some studies, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain has also shown similarities between CFS and post-polio fatigue. Areas of hyperintense signal in gray and white matter were imaged in 55% of polio survivors reporting at least moderate daily fatigue but were not imaged in any subjects reporting mild fatigue. 42 Small discrete or multiple punctate areas of HS were imaged in the putamen, the rostral reticular formation, centrum semiovale and periventricular and deep white matter. The presence of HS was significantly correlated with the severity of fatigue, mind wandering, and subjective difficulty thinking clearly, with attention, recent memory and concentration, but not with depressive symptoms or difficulty sleeping. These areas of HS were interpreted as evidence of poliovirus damage to the basal ganglia, RAS and its associated white matter tracts connecting the brain stem with the cortex. 2 This conclusion is supported by a recent case of poliomyelitis, in which David Bodian found HS in the midbrain and on antemortem MRI corresponded with histopathological findings of necrosis in the substantia nigra and reticular formation on autopsy. 43
Periventricular and deep white (but not gray) matter HS have also been imaged in 27% to 100% of CFS patients. 44- 45 White matter HS imaged in both demented and nondemented elderly adults have also been associated with impairments of attention and information processing speed similar to those documented in CFS patients and polio survivors. 46; cf. 52
Neuroendocrine data also show similarities between CFS and post- polio fatigue. 17 Plasma ACTH was significantly elevated and outside of the normal range following an overnight fast in polio survivors reporting mild daily fatigue (28.5 ± 17.7 ng/ml) but not in those reporting high (greater than moderate) fatigue (19.7 ± 10.7 ng/ml) (t=2.02; p<0.05). Plasma ACTH was also significantly negatively correlated with the frequency of problems with recent memory, word finding and muscle weakness, the severity of daily fatigue, recent memory and staying awake during the day, but not with the severity of depressive symptoms. Plasma cortisol levels were neither elevated nor different between subjects reporting mild (14.8 ± 5.7 ug/dl) and high daily fatigue (12.6 ± 5.2 ug/dl).
These data suggest that the HPA axis response to stress is blunted in polio survivors reporting fatigue. Decreased HPA activity has already been documented in patients with CFS and the reduced secretion of "activating" peptides such as CRH and ACTH has been implicated in its pathophysiology. 53-54
Polioencephalitis as a Prototype for PVFS
These data suggest that poliovirus may be the prototype for a chronic fatigue-producing agent since it routinely and often preferentially damaged areas responsible for brain activation and attention and has been so often associated during this century with symptoms of acute and chronic fatigue. Certainly, poliovirus is not the only agent for which the brain's activating system is its "favourite location." Both gray and white matter lesions have been demonstrated with a variety of viral encephalitides whose symptoms include markedly impaired cortical activation, e.g., St. Louis, Japanese B, Equine and Australian X encephalitis and Central European Encephalomyelitis. 55-56 Still other agents (e.g., Coxsackie, echo and herpes viruses) have been associated with symptoms of chronic fatigue. 18-20 And, although there is no convincing clinical or immunological evidence that post-polio fatigue or CFS is caused by a persistent poliovirus infection, only poliovirus has been directly or indirectly associated so often throughout this century with acute and chronic impairment of cortical activation, decreased attention and symptoms of fatigue. 57- 58 (Explanations for the recrudescence, de novo and stress induced appearance of post-polio fatigue are presented elsewhere. 2,17)
Polioencephalitis and the Brain Fatigue Generator. The findings presented above describe an integral relationship between polioencephalitis, decreased cortical activation and impaired attention. However, impaired attention is not fatigue’s only symptom. Even more disabling is the "visceral" experience of fatigue: feelings of exhaustion, "passivity and an aversion to continued effort" that generate an antipathy toward both mental and physical activity. 59,60 However unpleasant and disabling these feelings are in humans, passivity and aversion to activity have clear survival value, especially in animals without conscious awareness that attention and information processing speed are impaired. An animal that continues to explore its environment even though its attention is impaired would be less able to direct and sustain attention on the goal of its exploration (e.g., searching for food) and would waste already diminishing energy stores. More importantly, impaired attention and information processing speed could also render an animal unaware of or slow to respond to dangers in its environment (e.g., a predator stalking the animal in search of its food). Thus, there would be survival value in a "Brain Fatigue Generator" that monitors cortical activation and biases the organism toward cessation of motor behavior and promotes sleep when attention and information processing ability are impaired. To explain pathological fatigue, some agent would have to impair a putative Brain Fatigue Generator by damaging brain areas responsible not only for cortical activation and attention, but also for behavioral activation and intentioned movement.
Putamen as Cortical Monitor. The lenticular nuclei are uniquely situated to act as the Brain Fatigue Generator (BFG), which monitors the level of cortical activation, stops an organism from moving and promotes sleep when its attention is inadequate to allow efficient and safe motor behavior. All cortical areas project to the dorsal striatum, especially the putamen which receives 33% of its innervation from the cortex. 61 The dorsal striatum is said to accumulate samples of ongoing cortical projected activity via glutaminergic cortical efferents that synapse with the distal dendritic spines of putamenal GABAergic medium spiny neurons (MSN). 62-66 The putamen also sends excitatory efferents to the descending reticular formation (dRF) that increase tonic motor unit activity and muscle tone. 61,65,67 Muscle tone is important not only in preparing an animal for movement, but also in activating the cortex via muscle spindle afferents that stimulate the ascending reticular formation (aRF). 61,68,69 Decreased muscle tone, induced by curarization, has been shown to produce cortical synchronization and "drowsiness" and even induce a level of anesthesia sufficient to perform surgery. 70- 72
Animals with lesions of the putamen demonstrate "difficulty transferring attention from one object to another," “apathy,” "hypokinesia" and "insensitiv[ity] to quite gross stimuli." 62 Electrical stimulation at frequencies above 20 Hz. of s ingle loci in the cat putamen causes cortical EEG desynchronization, opening of the eyelids and dilatation of the pupils. However, stimulation of those same loci at frequencies below 9 Hz. causes cortical EEG synchronization, "closing of the eyelids accompanied by a narrowing of the pupils." 61 Repeated or long term low frequency stimulation of the putamen causes motor inhibition where all spontaneous activity stops and the animal remains motionless. "In this sleep-like state the eyes will be closed or half closed, even when the animal is not lying down." 61
These findings suggest that the integrity of putamenal neurons and the rate at which corticostriatal efferents stimulate them regulate the state of activation of the animal in terms of its ability to direct attention, perform motor behavior and even maintain wakefulness. 73,74
Pallidum as Phasic Moderator of Activation. The putamen's regulation of cortical and motor activation is accomplished directly through its inputs to the pallidum. Two types of putamenal medium spiny neurons (MSN) receive cortical efferents and project to the pallidum: Neurons expressing dopamine 1 (D1) receptors on their distal dendritic spines project to and inhibit the firing of the internal globus pallidus (GPi), while neurons expressing dopamine 2 (D2) receptors on their distal dendritic spines project to and inhibit the firing of the external globus pallidus (GPe). 61,64
Putamenal neurons projecting to the GPi are also thought to send inhibitory collaterals to the proximal dendritic shafts of putamenal GPe inhibitory neurons. 65,75 The lateral inhibition of GPe inhibitory neurons by MSN that inhibit the GPi is the simplest explanation for electrical stimulation of the putamen (but not the GPi) producing cortical and behavioral activation identical to that achieved by direct stimulation of GPe. 61
Putamenal inhibition of the pallidum has two opposite effects. Inhibition of the GPi reduces the firing of its GABAergic efferents which tonically inhibit the ventral lateral and ventral anterior thalamic nuclei. 61-76 When disinhibited, these thalamic nuclei allow cortical desynchronization and send an "enabling" signal to the supplementary motor and premotor motor areas, allowing the initiation of intentional movement and the execution of "learned motor plans." 61-64,77-79 Putamenal inhibition of the GPe reduces the tonic firing of its efferents which disinhibits the subthalamic nucleus (STN), allowing the STN to stimulate the GPi to inhibit the thalamus. Reduced GPe firing also decreases stimulation of the descending reticular formation (dRF) and reduces muscle tone. 61,65,67 The profound consequence of reduced GPe firing is seen in the akinetic mutism resulting from GPe lesions. 62
Substantia Nigra as Tonic Moderator of Activation. The release of dopamine from the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) increases the level of excitability of putamenal GPi inhibitory neurons via stimulation of D1 receptors, and inhibits putamenal GPe inhibitory neurons via D2 receptor stimulation. 64,65 The SNc also sends excitatory inputs to the dRF that enhance muscle tone. 61 Decreased dopaminergic input to the putamen in humans decreases the diffuse activation of the cortex and the ability to maintain targeted attention and even produces akinesia - symptoms that can be reversed to some extent by the administration of L-Dopa or dopamine receptor agonists. 81,82 A reduction in dopaminergic input to the lenticular nucleus in Parkinson's disease (PD) has been implicated as the cause of hypokinesia, bradykinesia, and the impaired ability to "transfer attention." 81,82
It is noteworthy that fatigue itself is also a prominent (albeit infrequently described) symptom of PD. 83 "Excessive fatigue" was reported by 48% of PD patients in one study, while nearly one-third of PD patients reported that fatigue was their "most disabling symptom." 83,84 One of the first descriptions of cognitive dysfunction in PD could serve as a definition of post-viral fatigue, i.e., a syndrome characterized by a diminution of voluntary attention, spontaneous interest, initiative and the capacity for effort and work, with significant and objective fatiguability, and a slight diminution of memory. 85
Physiology of Non Pathological Fatigue. Fatigue may occur normally when a reduction in the activation of the cortex decreases the firing of neurons connecting cortex to the putamen. The resulting decrease in putamen MSN firing would decrease inhibition of the GPi, permit continued inhibition of the GPe and decrease stimulation of the dRF, allowing a reduction in muscle tone and reduced aRF stimulation of the cortex. Inhibition of the GPe would disinhibit the STN, allowing it to stimulate the GPi to inhibit thalamic nuclei, prevent release of the motor activating set, halt execution of learned motor plans, stop intentional movements, increase cortical synchronization and promote sleep in animals who have no conscious awareness of fatigue. Humans would subjectively perceive these events as the "aversion to effort, feelings of passivity and difficulty staying awake that are symptoms of fatigue. 86 Inhibition of the motor activating set and decreased dRF activity could also generate two of the peripheral signs of central fatigue - the relaxation and lack of recruitment of motor units - that may underlie the visceral feeling of exhaustion that accompanies fatigue. 86 In both humans and animals, rest or sleep would restore cortical activation and stimulation of the putamen, which would inhibit the GPi, disinhibit the GPe, release the motor activating set, and restore activity within the reticular formation to reinforce cortical activation, release learned motor behavior and allow intentional movement.
Pathophysiology of Post-Polio Fatigue. Polio survivors' chronic fatigue, or the excessive fatigue that follows mild activity or emotional stress, may result from poliovirus lesions in brain biasing the BFG in favor of the fatigue generating process described above. A reduction cortical activation may result from poliovirus damage to neurons in the motor and premotor areas decreasing the firing of cortical efferents to the putamen. Poliovirus lesions in the putamen, globus pallidus, thalamus and midbrain reticular formation may also:
Reduce putamenal inhibition of the GPi and prevent disinhibition of the GPe (allowing chronic STN stimulation of the GPi);
Prevent thalamic nuclei from stimulating motor and prefrontal cortices; and
Decrease stimulation of the dRF, reduce muscle tone and thereby decrease muscle spindle efferent activation of the aRF and the cortex.
These impairments of phasic activation of the brain may be superimposed on impairment of tonic activation of the putamen and dRF resulting from poliovirus lesion in the substantia nigra. A decrease in dopaminergic input could bias the putamen in favor of disinhibiting the GPi (as a result of decreased stimulation of excitatory D1 receptors on GPi inhibitory neurons) and maintaining inhibition of the GPe (as a result of decreased stimulation of inhibitory D2 receptors on GPe inhibitory neurons). Decreased SNc output would also reduce dRF stimulation, decrease muscle tone and aRF activity, as well as reduce direct dopaminergic stimulation of the cortex. 87 SPECT scans in patients with ME showing significantly decreased perfusion of the brain stem have provided the first evidence that a decrease in the activity of brain stem (possibly reticular formation) neurons is associated with chronic fatigue. 88
These putative effects of poliovirus lesions in the motor cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus and reticular formation may be reinforced by poliovirus-lesions in other areas of the CNS. White matter hyperintensities on MRI in polio survivors reporting fatigue may be evidence of damage to myelinated fibers, or their wallerian degeneration due to gray matter lesions, which may compromise transmission between or partially disconnect the thalamus and the cortex. 52,89,90 The decreased secretion of ACTH in polio survivors with fatigue, possibly resulting from poliovirus lesions within the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) or pallidal neurons projecting to the PVN, may also contribute to a generalized impairment of brain activation. 17,91 Polio survivors, regardless of their level of fatigue, have been shown in two separate studies to have an impairment of visual memory, a function ascribed to the right hemisphere (RH). 40 Since the RH is thought to play a central role in both cortical activation and preparation for movement, and right hemisphere lesions are associated with reduced cortical and behavioral activation, hypokinesia and even akinesia, polio survivors may have a hemispheric predisposition to decreased cortical activation and fatigue. 40,92,93 Finally, ubiquitous damage to anterior horn motor neurons may tonically reduce muscle tone, muscle spindle afferent stimulation of the aRF, predisposing polio survivors to decreased cortical activation and fatigue. 12
Treatment of Post-Polio Fatigue with a Dopamine Receptor Agonist
The central role of the basal ganglia in the putative Brain Fatigue Generator suggests that stimulation of its dopaminergic efferents might increase cortical activation, counter attentional impairments, release the motor activating set and reduce the symptoms of fatigue. Therefore, bromocriptine mesylate (Parlodel; Sandoz), a direct acting, post synaptic dopamine 2 (D2) receptor agonist, was chosen for a placebo-controlled pilot study of a pharmacological treatment for post-polio fatigue. Eighty-three polio survivors (who were without comorbidities that could cause fatigue or cognitive problems) completed treatment at the Post-Polio Service and complied with conservative therapies for fatigue (energy conservation, work simplification, pacing and daily rest periods.) Regardless of the severity of their fatigue before treatment, only 10 (12%) reported a typical daily fatigue level greater than moderate (the inclusion criterion for the study) after treatment. Two patients were diagnosed as having a major depressive episode and five of the remaining patients agreed to participate in the drug study. Four of those subjects rated their usual daily fatigue as severe and were on Social Security disability as a result of fatigue preventing continued employment. The remaining subject rated her daily fatigue as moderate but was only able to work part-time.
Subjects were given log forms and instructed to rate their fatigue upon awakening, at 12:00, 3:00, 6:00 and 9:00 PM. At 9:00 PM, subjects were instructed to describe any side effects experienced during the day and rate their overall difficulty with motivation, attention, mind wandering, thinking clearly, concentration, word finding, object naming, memory, staying awake during the day, and muscle weakness.
Subjects were given placebo for 28 days and then increasing doses of bromocriptine for 28 days up to a maximum of 12.75 mg/day. Three subjects reported a noticeable reduction in fatigue and related cognitive symptoms on bromocriptine as compared to placebo. These drug responders reported that they "felt awake" and "had a clear head" for the first time in many years; their daily symptoms logs documented decreased difficulty with attention and other cognitive symptoms, motivation and morning fatigue that were significantly correlated with days on bromocriptine, but not with days on placebo.
Drug responders had more severe CNS abnormalities at baseline than did non-responders; they had more than twice as many hyperintensities on MRI of the brain, a mean plasma ACTH that did not increase following an overnight fast and clinically abnormal scores on neuropsychological tests of attention (Trail Making Test (Part A)), information processing speed (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (2.4 numbers/second presentation speed)), distractibility and vigilance (Gordon Diagnostic System). 94 A resting mean prolactin level nearly double that of the non-responders suggested chronically reduced dopaminergic inhibition of prolactin secretion in the drug responders. These preliminary findings support the BFG model that suggests inhibition of putamenal MSN that inhibit the GPe, via stimulation of their D2 receptors, may reduce symptoms of fatigue. This conclusion is also supported by a placebo-controlled study of healthy subjects who were administered remoxipride, a potent and selective D2 receptor antagonist. 95 The most frequently reported effects of D2 blockade were "moderate fatigue," "mild somnolence" and "difficulty concentrating." Statistically significant, dose-related increases in subjective "drowsiness" and scores on neuropsychological tests of auditory vigilance, continuous attention and critical flicker fusion were also found following D2 blockade.
We are continuing to test the Brain Fatigue Generator model by measuring attention, EEG power spectra and plasma prolactin in polio survivors reporting chronic fatigue. We are just beginning studies of patients with CFS using this same protocol to determine if the BFG model is applicable to the pathophysiology of CFS.
References
1 Bruno RL, Frick NM. Stress and "Type A" behavior as precipitants of Post-Polio Sequelae. In: Halstead LS, Wiechers DO, eds. Research and Clinical Aspects of the Late Effects of Poliomyelitis. White Plains, NY: March of Dimes; 1987:145-155.
2 Bruno RL, Frick NM, Cohen J. Polioencephalitis, stress and the etiology of post-polio sequelae. Orthopedics. 1991; 14:1185-93.
3 Holmgren BE. Electro-encephalography in poliomyelitis. Poliomyelitis. Philadelphia: Lippincott; 1952:448-450.
4 Meyer E. Psychological considerations in a group of children with poliomyelitis. J Pediatrics. 1947; 31: 34-48.
5 Bodian D. Poliomyelitis: Neuropathologic observations in relation to motor symptoms. J Am Med Assoc. 1947; 134: 1148-1154.
6 Howe HA , Bodian D. Neural Mechanisms of Poliomyelitis. New York: The Commonwealth Fund;
1942.
7 Brown JR, Baker AB, McQuarrie I, et al. The bulbar form of poliomyelitis. JAMA. 1947; 135: 425- 428.
8 Baker AB. Neurologic signs of bulbar poliomyelitis. Poliomyelitis. Philadelphia: Lippincott; 1949: 241-244
9 Howe HA, Bodian D. Neuropathological evidence on the portal of entry problem in human poliomyelitis. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp. 1941; 69: 183- 214.
10 Bodian D, Howe HA. Neurotropism and the genesis of cerebral lesions in poliomyelitis. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp. 1941; 68: 58-76.
11 Guizetti HU. Betrachtungen zur poliomyelitis des hirnstammes. Deutsch Ztschr f Nervenh. 1933:131: 29-42.
12 Bodian D. Histopathological basis of clinical findings in poliomyelitis. Am J Med. 1949; 6: 563-578.
13 Barnhart M, Rhines R, McCarter JC, Magoun HW. Distribution of lesions of the brain stem in poliomyelitis. Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1948; 59: 368-377.
14 Matzke H A , Baker AB. Poliomyelitis: A study of the midbrain. Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1951; 65: 1-15.
15 Peers JH. The pathology of convalescent poliomyelitis in man. Am J Pathol. 1942; 19: 673-695.
16 Luhan JA. Epidemic poliomyelitis: Some pathological observations on human material. Arch Pathol. 1946; 42: 245- 260.
17 Bruno RL, Sapolsky R, Zimmerman JR, Frick NM. The pathophysiology of a central cause of post-polio fatigue. Ann NY Acad Sci. 1995; 753: 257 - 275.
18 Behan PO, Behan WMH. Postviral fatigue syndrome. CRC Critical Reviews in Neurobiology. 1988; 4: 157-178.
19 Buchwald DPR, Cheney PR, Peterson DL, et al. A chronic illness characterized by fatigue, neurologic and immunologic disorders and active human herpesvirus type 6 infection. Ann Int Med. 1992; 116:103-113.
20 Hyde BM. Myalgic Encephalomyelitis (Chronic Fatigue Syndrome): An historical perspective. In: Hyde BM, Goldstein J, Levine P, eds. The Clinical and Scientific Basis of Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Ottawa: The Nightingale Research Foundation. 1992: 111- 118.
21 Acheson ED. The clinical syndrome variously called benign myalgic encephalomyelitis, Iceland Disease and epidemic neuromyasthenia. Am J Med. 1959; 26: 569-595.
22 Macrae AD, Galpine JF. An illness resembling poliomyelitis observed in nurses. Lancet. 1954; ii: 350-352.
23 Gilliam AG. Epidemiological study of an epidemic, diagnosed as poliomyelitis, occurring among the personnel of the Los Angeles County General Hospital during the summer of 1934. US Public Health Bull. 1938; (No. 240): 1- 90.
24 Gsell VO. Abortive poliomyelitis. Helvetica Medica Acta. 1949; 3/4:169-183.
25 Fog T. Vegetative (epidemic?) neuritis. Ugeskr Laeg. 1953; 115: 1244-1248.
26 Pellew RA. Clinical description of a disease resembling poliomyelitis seen in Adelaide. Med J Aust. 1951; 1: 944-946.
27 White DN, Burtch RB. Iceland Disease: A new infection simulating acute anterior poliomyelitis. Neurology. 1954; 4: 506-516.
28 Deisher JB. Benign myalgic encephalomyelitis (Iceland Disease) in Alaska. Northwest Med. 1957; 56: 1451-1456.
29 Sigurdsson B, Gudmundsson KR. Clinical findings six years after outbreak of Akureyri Disease. Lancet. 1957; i: 766-767.
30 Sigurdsson B, Sigurjonsson J, Sigurdsson HJ, et al. A disease epidemic in Iceland simulating poliomyelitis. Am J Hyg. 1950;52:222-238.
31 Sigurdsson B, Gudnadottir M, Petursson G. Response to poliomyelitis vaccination. Lancet. 1958; I: 370-371.
32 Hyde BM, Jain A. Clinical observations of central nervous system dysfunction in post-infectious, acute onset M.E./CFS. In: Hyde BM, Goldstein J, Levine P, eds. The Clinical and Scientific Basis of Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Ottawa: The Nightingale Research Foundation. 1992: 38-65.
33 Altay HT, Toner BB, Brooker H, et al. The neuropsychological dimensions of post infectious neuromyesthenia. Intl J Psychiatry in Med. 1990; 20:141- 149.
34 Jamal GA. Evidence for organic disturbance in Post Viral Fatigue Syndrome. In: Hyde BM, Goldstein J, Levine P, eds. The Clinical and Scientific Basis of Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Ottawa: The Nightingale Research Foundation. 1992: 414-424.
35 Ramsay AM, OSullivan E. Encephalomyelitis simulating poliomyelitis. Lancet. 1956; i: 762-767.
36 Galpine JF, Brady C. Benign myalgic encephalomyelitis. Lancet. 1957; i:757-758.
37 DeLuca J, Johnson SK, Natelson BH. Information processing efficiency in chronic fatigue syndrome and multiple sclerosis. Arch Neurol. 1993; 50: 301-304.
38 Sandman CA, Barron JL, Nackoul K, et al. Memory deficits associated with chronic fatigue immune dysfunction syndrome. Biol Psychiatry. 1993; 33:618-623.
39 Graffman J, Schwartz V, Dale JK, et al. Analysis of neuropsychological functioning in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Neurol, Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1991; 56:684-689.
40 Bruno RL, Galski T, DeLuca J. Neuropsychology of PostPolio Fatigue Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1993; 74: 1061-1065.
41 McDonald E, Cope H, David A. Cognitive impairment in patients with chronic fatigue. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1993; 56:812-815.
42 Bruno RL, Cohen J, Galski T, Frick NM. The neuroanatomy of post- polio fatigue. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1994; 74:1061-1065.
43 Wasserstrom R, mamourian AC, McGary CT, Miller G. Bulbar poliomyslitis: MR findings with pathological correlation. AJNR. 1992; 13:371-373.
44 Natelson BH, Cohen JM, Brassloff, Lee, H-J. A controlled study of brain magnetic resonance imaging in patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome. J Neurological Sci. 1993; 120:213-217.
45 Daugherty SA, Henry BE, Peterson DL, et al. Chronic fatigue syndrome in northern Nevada. Rev Infect Dis. 1991; 13 (Suppl 1):S39-44.
46 Almkvist O, Wahlund L-O, Anderson-Lundman G, et al. White-matter hyperintensity and neuropsychological functions in dementia and healthy aging. Arch Neurol. 1992; 49:626-632.
47 Kertesz A, Polk M, Carr T. Cognition and white matter changes on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia. Arch Neurol. 1990; 47:387-391.
48 Kirkpatrick JB, Hayman LA. White-matter lesions in MR imaging of clinically healthy brains of elderly subjects: Possible pathological basis. Radiology. 1987; 162:509-511.
49 Austron MG, Thompson RF, Hendrie HC, et al. Foci of increased T2 signal intensity in MR images of healthy elderly subjects: A follow-up study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1990; 38:1133-1138.
50 Boone KB, Miller BL, Lesser IM, et al. Neuropsychological correlates of white-matter lesions in healthy elderly subjects. Arch. Neurol. 1992; 49:549-554.
51 Junque C, Pujol J, Vendrell P, et al. Leuko-araiosis on magnetic resonance imaging and speed of mental processing. Arch Neurol. 1990; 47:151-156.
52 Breteler MMB, van Amerongen NM, van Swieten JC, et al. Cognitive correlates of ventricular enlargement and vertebral white matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging: The Rotterdam study. Stroke. 1994; 25: 1109-1115.
53 Demitrack MA, Dale JK, Straus SE, et al. Evidence for impaired activation of the hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal axis in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Clin Endocrinology Metabolism. 1991; 73: 1224-1234.
54 Poteliakhoff A. Adrenocortical activity and some clinical findings in acute and chronic fatigue. J Psychosom Res. 1981; 25:91-95.
55 Magoun HW. Poliomyelitis. Philadelphia: Lippincott. 1949: 250. 56 Grinschgl G , Richling E. Clinical, pathologic and epidemiologic criteria of Central European Encephalomyelitis (CEE) and its differential diagnosis for poliomyelitis. Poliomyelitis. Philadelphia: Lippincott. 1955: 417-490.
57 Bruno RL. Post-Polio Sequelae. Orthopedics. 1991; 14:1169-1170.
58 Melchers W, De Visser M, Jongen P, et al. The postpolio syndrome: No evidence for poliovirus persistence. Ann Neurol. 1992; 32:728-732.
59 Hockey GRJ. Changes in operator efficiency as a function of environmental stress, fatigue and circadian rhythms. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L, Thomas JP, eds. Handbook of Perception and Human Performance (Vol 2.). New York: John Wiley & Sons;1986: 197-199.
60 Robbins TW. Psychopharmacological and neurobiological aspects of the energetics of information processing. In: Hockey GRJ, Gaillard AWK, Coles MGH, eds. Energetics and Human Information Processing. Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff; 1986:71-89.
61 Hassler R. Striatal control of locomotion, intentional actions and if integrating and perceptive activity. J Neurol Sci. 1978; 36:187-224.
62 Denny-Brown D, Yanagisawa N. The role of the basal ganglia in the initiation of movement. In: Yahr MD, ed. The Basal Ganglia. New York: Raven; 1976:115-149.
63 Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1986; 9:357-381.
64 Strange PG. Dopamine receptors in the basal ganglia. Movement Disorders. 1993; 8:263-270.
65 Lindefors N. Dopaminergic regulation of glutamic acid decarboxylase mRNA expression and GABA release in the striatum: A review. Prog Neuro-Pharmacol & Biol Psychiat. 1993; 17:887-903.
66 Smith AD, Bolam JP. The neural network of the basal ganglia as revealed by the study of synaptic connections of identified neurons. TINS. 1990; 13:260-265.
67 Watson RT, Valenstein E, Heilman KM. Thalamic neglect: Possible role of the medial thalamus and nucleus reticularis in behavior. Arch Neurol. 1981; 38:501-506.
68 Gellhorn E. Autonomic imbalance and the hypothalamus. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press: 1957.
69 Kleitman N. Sleep and wakefulness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1939.
70 Gellhorn E. The influence of curare on hypothalamic excitability and the electroencephalogram. EEG clin Neurophysiol 1958; 10: 697-703.,
71 Hodes R. Electrocortical synchronization resulting from reduced proprioceptive drive caused by neuromuscular blocking agents. EEG clin Neurophysiol 1962: 14: 220-232.,
72 Shea LT, Davidson GM, Davis J. Electroencephalographic studies of the curarized patient. Med J Aust. 1954; 2: 656-659.
73 Owen AM, Roberts AC, Hodges JR, et al. Contrasting mechanisms if impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinsons disease. Brain. 1993; 116:1159-1175.
74 Early TS, Posner MI, Reiman EM, Raichle ME. Hyperactivity of the left striato-pallidal projection. Part I: Lower level theory. Psychiatric Developments. 1989; 2:85- 108.
75 Groves PM. A theory of the functional organization of the neostriatum and the neostriatal control of voluntary movement. Brain Res Rev. 1983; 5:109-132.
76 Alexander GE, Crutcher MD. Functional architecture of basal ganglia circuits: Neural substrates of parallel processing. TINS. 1990; 13: 266- 271.
77 Early TS, Posner MI, Reiman EM, Raichle ME. Left striato-pallidal hyperactivity in schizophrenia Part II: Phenomenology and thought disorder. Psychiatric Developments. 1989; 2:109-121.
78 Schell GR, Strick PL. The origin of thalamic inputs to the arcuate premotor and supplementary motor areas. J. Neurosci. 1984; 2:539-560. 79 Chevalier G, Deniau JM. Disinhibition as a basic process in the expression of striatal function. TINS. 1990; 13:277- 280.
80 Echiverri HC., Tatum WO, Merens TA, Coker SB. Akinetic mutism. Pediatric Neurol. 1988; 4:228-230.
81 Bowen FP. Behavioral alterations in patients with basal ganglia lesions. Research Publications: Assoc Res Nerv Ment Dis. 1976; 55:169- 180.
82 Brown RG, Marsden CD. Cognitive function in Parkinsons disease. TINS. 1990; 1:21-28.
83 Friedman J, Friedman H. Fatigue in Parkinsons disease. Neurology. 1993; 43:2016-2018.
84 van Hilten JJ, Hoogland G, van der Velde EA, et al. Diurnal effects of motor activity and fatigue in Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1993; 56:874-877.
85 Naville F. Encephale. 1922; 17:369-375.
86 Edwards RHT. Human muscle function and fatigue. Human Muscle Fatigue. Ciba Foundation Symposium 82. London: Pittman; 1981.
87 Stam CJ, Visser SL, Op de Coul, AAW, et al. Disturbed frontal regulation of attention in Parkinsons disease. Brain. 1993; 116:1139- 1158.
88 Costa DC, Tannoc C, Brostoff J. Brainstem perfusion is impaired in chronic fatigue syndrome. Q. J. Med. 1995; 88:767-773.
89 Leys D, Pruvo JP, Parent, M, et al. Could Wallerian degeneration contribute to "leuko-ariosis" is subjects free of any vascular disorder? J. Neuro. Neurosurg. Psychiat. 1991; 5: 46-50.